Charles
Salter
SALTER,
FALL, 2003
If
women do have a tendency to elicit high self-monitoring behavior
because they have a need to get along with others, and if men elicit
a high self-monitoring behavior because they have an acquisitive
disposition, an acknowledgement of those apposed motivations would
lead to a better understanding of the diverse talents that each group
brings to the job.
In
1972, researchers reported that only 4% of the Master of Business
Administration graduates were women. This figure now exceeds 33%.
Similarly, in 1972, women occupied about 20% of non-clerical, white-collar
jobs. This figure has now grown to more than 46% (Sharpe, 1994).
Sharpe also contended that in 1972, only 17% of all managerial
positions were held by women, but by 1994 that number had increased
by 43%. Although women have flooded managerial positions in recent
years, concern still remains about what some refer to as a "glass
ceiling" that prohibits women from reaching the topmost levels
of corporate leadership. Ragins, Townsend, and Mattis (1998) stated
that, of the most highly compensated corporate executives in Fortune
500 companies only 2% are currently women and only .4% of the Fortune
1000 Chief Executive Officer, (CEO), positions are held by women.
Maruca (1997) promulgated that male CEOs blamed the "glass
ceiling" on women's lack of experience and time in organizations.
In the same study, however, female executives disagreed citing
exclusionary corporate cultures as the reason for their lack of
advancement to top management positions. This paper looks at one
of the human behaviors known to be compatible with emergent leadership,
self-monitoring behavior, and its possible relationship
to the "glass ceiling" prohibiting women's advancement
within corporations.
Literature
Review
Leadership
Emergence
Bass
(1981) made a major distinction in leadership research between
individuals who are formally appointed to positions of leadership
and individuals who emerge as leaders of formal groups. De Souza
and Klein (1995) concluded that groups with emergent leaders outperformed
groups without emergent leaders. Most of the research investigating
emergent leadership has been directed by the trait approach, which
assumes that leaders are endowed with certain innate characteristics
that predispose them to be effective leaders in a wide range of
situations. Empirical support for the trait approach has been lacking
and because of this some researchers have concluded that a leadership
trait does not exist (Jenkins, 1947). However, Lord, De Vader,
and Alliger (1986) found that some variance in leader emergence
could be predicted by the dominance, intelligence, and masculinity-femininity
of the leader. Hollander (1961;1964) identified task competence
and identification as traits of emergent leaders. Task competence
encompasses the set of characteristics that are required by a group
to attain its goals and includes social competencies. Identification
entails a clear involvement with the group task and loyalty toward
group members. Sorrentino and Boutillier (1975) found rate of verbal
participation to be a predictor of emergent leadership. Emergent
leadership has also been positively related to extroversion, openness
to experience, and cognitive ability (Kickul & Neuman, 2000).
Kenny and Zaccaro (1983) proposed that persons who are consistently
cast into leadership positions possess the ability to perceive
and predict variations in group situations and pattern their own
behavior accordingly. Dobbins, Long, Dedrick, and Clemons (1990)
concluded that Kenny and Zaccaro's (1983) characteristics of persons
regularly cast into leadership roles were consistent with Snyder's
(1979) description of self-monitoring behavior.
Self-Monitoring
Behavior
Snyder
(1974) related that people who engage in high self-monitoring behavior
regulate their behavior in ways that are highly sensitive to situational
cues. High self-monitors have a strong concern that their behavior
is appropriate for the social situations in which they find themselves.
Individuals who are high self-monitors are particularly sensitive
to the social cues and self-presentations of others, and use social
cues as guidelines for managing their own behavior and or creating
desirable impressions (Synder, 1986). In contrast, low self-monitoring
persons display less concern for the situational appropriateness
of their behavior, which appears to be guided from their internal
cues, rather than by situational specifications of appropriate
behavior. Therefore, one might say that high self-monitors are
impression managers who are to a great degree concerned with the
impression their actions have on others. Their concern for the
impression they make results in their adjusting their behavior
so as to present themselves in the most favorable light. Low self-monitors
are more inclined to act in accord with their own true feelings,
attitudes, and values in social settings (Synder, 1986). It has
been suggested by Turnley and Bolino (2001) that high self-monitors
elicit the same five personality characteristics as those reported
by Costa and McCrae (1988). The Big Five personality traits are
said to help predict a person's success in organizations. In summary
they are: 1) Extroversion- or the extent to which a person is outgoing,
2) Agreeableness-or the degree to which one is cooperative, 3)
Openness to experience-defines a nature to be curious and creative,
4) Emotional Stability-or a persons propensity to be calm, self
confident, and cool, and 5) Conscientiousness-the degree to which
an individual is hard working, dependable, and persevering. Barrick
and Mount (1991) reported that these personality characteristics
were valid predictors of success for many occupational groups.
Some
research has found a strong relationship between self-monitoring
and leader emergence in groups (Cronshaw & Ellis, 1991; Ellis,
Adamson, Deszca & Cawsey, 1988). Other research has suggested
that these effects are moderated by the sex of group members and
the nature of the task confronting the group (Ellis, 1988; Garland
& Beard, 1979). Ellis (1988) examined the effects of self-monitoring
on leader emergence in natural mixed-sex groups and found that
leader emergence and high self-monitoring behavior were related
for males, but not for females. Dobbins et al. (1990) found that
high self-monitoring men emerged as leaders in groups of high self-monitoring
women and men disproportionately to women. Nyquist and Spence (1986)
reported that 90% of high dominant men became leaders over low
dominant women, and only 25% of high dominant women emerged as
leaders over low dominant men. Wentworth and Anderson (1984) found
that men emerged as leaders in 86% of mixed-sex groups. Carbonell
(1984) showed that females with leadership ability assert leadership
in interactions with other females but fail to do so in the company
of males. It is plausible that high self-monitoring females in
mixed-sex groups inhibit leadership behavior because they fear
such behavior will be viewed as inappropriate according to sex
role stereotypes. In contrast to Ellis' (1988), finding that high
self-monitoring behavior correlated with leader emergence in males,
Garland and Beard (1979) found that high self-monitoring females,
but not males, emerged as leaders in their respective groups. These
results in regard to high self-monitoring females can be explained
by sex role research demonstrating that females will assert leadership
in interaction with other females (Carbonell, 1984; Megargee, 1969).
Kent and Moss (1994) found in their study on emergent leaders that
masculine subjects emerged as leaders more often than feminine
subjects. Another study indicated that sex differences in emergent
leadership are due to role-induced tendencies for men to specialize
more than women in behaviors strictly oriented to their group's
task and for women to specialize more than men in socially facilitative
behaviors (Eagly, Karau, & Steven, 1991; Karakowsky, Leonard,
& Siegel, 1999). Most if not all the traits that have been
studied about emergent leadership are characteristics of those
who are high self-monitors.
Other
differences in female and male high self-monitors have been documented.
Shaffer and Pegalis (1998) reported that high self-monitoring females
failed to elicit more self-disclosure than their low self-monitoring
counterparts, while male high self-monitors did elicit more self-disclosure.
Men high self-monitors clearly promoted male self-disclosure in
a collaborative or work setting context, whereas female high self-monitors
actually inhibited self-disclosure in collaborative work settings.
In contrast to women's reported tendency to self-disclose to other
women more in social-expressive contexts, Shaffer and Ogden (1986)
and Shaffer and Pegalis (1998) found that women high self-monitors
did not elicit more self-disclosure from female acquaintances in
social-expressive contexts than in collaborative contexts. Guarino,
Michael, and Hocevar (1997) reported differences in the social
integration of female high self-monitors and male high self-monitors
in relation to student integration into community college life.
Specifically, they found that male high self-monitors were socially
integrated faster and to a greater degree, than female high self-monitors.
The
questions that arise, are: What makes female high self-monitors
less likely to be emergent leaders than male high self-monitors
in mixed-sex groups? And what stimulates women high self-monitors
to be less likely to elicit self-disclosure from other women in
a collaborative work setting, and less socially integrating in
college environments? Briggs and Cheeks (1988) and Lennox and Wolfe
(1984) stated that people vary their self-presentation style for
different motives and that these motives should be taken into account
in studying self-presentation. According to Wolfe, Lennox, and
Cutler (1986), high self-monitors vary their self-presentation
for two reasons: 1) acquisition-or trying to get ahead, or 2) self-protection-trying
to get along. Research also discovered that the occurrence of protective
and acquisitive self-presentations is contingent on: the audience
of the presenter, the context, and the person making the self-presentation.
Wolfe et al. (1986) also reported that self-presenters chronically
adopt either a protective or an acquisitive self-presentation style.
He further reports that factors associated with self-presenters
adopting a protective style include: social anxiety, shyness, conformity,
reticence, low self-esteem, modesty, and neutrality. Conversely
people adopting an acquisitive self-presentation style are characterized
as being more self-confident and higher in self-esteem. Understandably
a high self-monitor that did not elicit traits found to be those
of emergent leaders, such as self-confidence, would probably be
less likely to be named as a leader by others.
Conclusion
In
conclusion more research should be done to identify the motivation
of women high self-monitors. Are those motivations concerned more
with the facilitative behaviors or trying to get along with others'
motivation rather than an
acquisitive desire. Also, more study should be conducted on the
differences in the men and women relate to tasks and group goals
and the way men and women engage in problem solving. One would
prefer to believe that male bias is not the reason for the "glass
ceiling" effect for women. One would further prefer to believe
that if this barrier does exist it is borne of a misunderstanding
of the differences in which men and women conduct conflict resolution
as well as goal setting and attainment. By understanding the motivations
of people, even if they are not similar to our own, we can understand
the principles on which they base decisions. By understanding the
logical reasoning constructs on which they base decisions we can
more easily predict their behavior. Being able to predict a manager's
behavior is a source of comfort to any superior and leads to a
better relationship and more fluidity of communication.
If
women do have a tendency to elicit high self-monitoring behavior
because they have a need to get along with others, and if men elicit
a high self-monitoring behavior because they have an acquisitive
disposition, an acknowledgement of those apposed motivations would
lead to a better understanding of the diverse talents that each
group brings to the job. We could then, as corporate managers,
make the best and highest use of all the personnel resources available
to us.
Today in corporate boardrooms throughout the United States, we
see companies that are finding themselves in one type of predicament
or another. Many of these problems are intensified because of a
collegiate atmosphere, or a reciprocity that emanates from like
thinking. This reciprocity results in board members being hesitant
to question the actions of those colleagues that are the Chief
Executive Officers of these corporations. For the above- mentioned
reason alone, we need to foster an understanding of the motivations
of men and women and welcome the difference, if one exists, into
a system mired down in like-mindedness.
References
Barrick,
M. R. & Mount, M. K. (1991). The big five personality dimensions
and job
performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 51,
577-598.
Bass,
B. (1981) . Stodgill's handbook of leadership. New York:
Free Press.
Briggs,
S. R., & Cheek, J. M. (1988). On the nature of self-monitoring:
Problems with assessment and problems with validity. Journal
of Personality and Social
Psychology, 54, 663-678.
Carbonell,
J. L. (1984). Sex roles and leadership revisited. Journal of
Applied
Psychology, 69, 44-49.
Costa,
P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). From catalog to classification:
Murray's needs and five-factor model. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 54, 258-265.
Cronshaw,
S. F., & Ellis, R. J. (1991). A process investigation of self-monitoring
and leader emergence. Small Group Research, 22, 403-420.
De
Souza, G., & Klein, H.J. (1995). Emergent leadership in the
group goal setting
process. Small Group Research, 26, 475-499.
Dobbins,
G. H., Long, W. S., Dedrick, E. J., Clemons, T. C. (1990). The
Role of self-monitoring and gender on leader emergence: A laboratory
and field study. Journal of Management, 16 (3), 609-618.
Eagly,
A. H., & Karau, S. J. (1991). Gender and the emergence of leaders:
A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,
60, 685-710.
Ellis,
R. J. (1988) . Self-monitoring and leadership emergence in groups.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 681-693.
Ellis,
R. J., Adamson, R. S., Deszca, G., & Cawsey, T. F. (1988).
Self-monitoring and leadership emergence. Small Group Research,
19, 312-324.
Garland,
H., & Beard, J. F. (1979). Relationship between self-monitoring
and leader emergence across two task situations. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 64, 72-76.
Guarino,
A., Michael, W. B., & Hocevar, D. (1997). Self-monitoring and
student
integration of community college students. Journal of Social
Psychology, 138, 754-760.
Hollander,
E. P. (1961). Emergent Leadership and social influence. Leadership
and
interpersonal behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Hollander,
E. P. (1964). Leaders, groups, and influence. New York:
Oxford University Press.
Jenkins,
W. O. (1947). A review of leadership studies with particular reference
to
military problems. Psychological Bulletin, 44, 54-79.
Karakowski,
L., & Siegel, J. P. (1999). The effects of proportional representation
and gender orientation of the task on emergent leadership behavior
in mixed-gender work groups. Journal of Applied Psychology,
84, 620-631.
Kenny,
D., & Zaccaro, S. (1983). An estimate of variance due to traits
in leadership. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 678-685.
Kent,
R. L., & Moss, S. E. (1994). Effects of sex and gender role
on leader emergence. Academy of Management Journal, 37,
1335-1346.
Kickul,
J., & Neuman, G. (2000). Emergent leadership behaviors: the
function of
personality and cognitive ability in determining. Journal of
Business &
Psychology, 15, 27-52.
Lennox,
R., & Wolfe, R. (1984). Revision of the self-monitoring scale.
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1349-1364.
Lord,
R. G., De Vader, C., & Alliger, G. M. (1986). A meta-analysis
of the relation
between personality traits and leadership perceptions: An application
of validity generalization procedures. Journal of Applied Psychology,
71, 402-410.
Maruca,
R. F. (1997). Says Who? Harvard Business Review, 12, 15-17.
Megargee,
E. I. (1969). Influence of sex roles on the manifestation of leadership.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 53, 377-382.
Nyquist,
L. V., & Spence, J. T. (1986). Effects of dispositional dominance
and sex role expectations on leadership behaviors. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 87-93.
Ragins,
B. R., Townsend, B., & Mantis, M. (1998). Gender gap in the
executive suite: CEOs and female executives report on breaking
the glass ceiling. Academy of Management Executive, 2, 28-42.
Shaffer,
D. R., & Ogden, J. K. (1986). On sex differences in self-disclosure
during the acquaintance process: The role of anticipated future
interaction. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 76-96.
Shaffer,
D. R., & Pegalis, L. J. (1998). Gender and situational context
moderate the relationship between self-monitoring and induction
of self-disclosure. Journal of Personality, 66, 215-234.
Sharpe,
R. (1994). Women make strides, but men stay firmly in top company
jobs. The Wall Street Journal, March 29, 1994.
Sorrentino,
R. M., & Boutillier, R. G. (1975). The effect of quantity and
quality verbal interaction on ratings of leadership ability. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 403-411.
Synder,
M. (1974). The self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 526-537.
Synder,
M. (1979). Self-monitoring processes. Advances in experimental
social
psychology, 12, 86-128.
Synder,
M. (1986). Public appearances/Private realities. New York:
Freeman and
Company.
Turnley,
W. H., & Bolino, M. C. (2001). Achieving desired images while
avoiding undesired images: Exploring the role of self-monitoring
in impression management. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86,
351-360.
Wentworth,
D. K., & Anderson, L. R. (1984). Emergent leadership as a function
of sex and type. Sex Roles, 11, 513-524.
Wolfe,
R., Lennox, R., & Cutler, B. (1986). "Getting along"
and "getting ahead":
Empirical support for a theory of protective and acquisitive self-presentation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 356-361.
Author
Charles Salter
received his Masters in Business Administration from the University
of Houston, TX and is currently working on his Ph.D. in Leadership
Studies at Our Lady of the Lake University in San Antonio, TX.
|